Effects of Cow Comfort on Milk Quality, Productivity, and Behavior


Introduction

Overstocking of freestall barns, defined as housing more cows within a pen than the available number of stalls and (or) providing less than the recommended 0.6 m (23 in.) of linear feeding space per cow (Grant and Albright, 2001), is a practice commonly employed by dairy producers to expand herd size without increasing the facility investment (Bewley et al., 2001). The short- and long-term effects of this practice on dairy cow behavior, productivity, and health are not fully understood. However, third-party auditing organizations such as Humane Farm Animal Care (www.certifiedhumane.org) currently require that member farms limit the number of cows within a pen to a number less than or equal to the number of freestalls. The current draft of the Dairy Well-Being Initiative (www.dairywellbeing.org), which represents the dairy industry’s consensus on animal welfare, contains language that would require adequate resting space to be available for all animals housed within a pen. The basis for these recommendations is the recognition that the health, productivity, and welfare of dairy cows rely on their ability to meet their behavioral needs each day.

Please check this link first if you are interested in organic or specialty dairy production.

The behavior of dairy cows depends on the interaction between the cows and their physical environment. In the “big picture,” the physical factors of the facility (stall design, flooring type, feed bunk design, environmental quality) impose baseline limitations on how the cows interact with the housing conditions. Within these limitations, the ability of cows to engage in natural behaviors is further dictated by management routines such as grouping strategy and stocking density. This paper will emphasize the effects of stocking density.

The natural behaviors that are most important to the health, welfare, and productivity of cows are resting, feeding, and rumination. Each of these is discussed in separate subsections below. In general, there has been a demonstrated need of 12 to 14 hours of resting time and 3 to 5 hours of feeding time per day (Table 1). These time budget requirements constitute 60% to 80% of a 24-hour period, which leaves a limited number of hours for milking and other management procedures.

Table 1. Daily time budget for lactating dairy cow (Grant, 2007).
Activity Time devoted to activity per day
Eating 3 to 5 h (9 to 14 meals/d)
Lying/resting 12 to 14 h
Social interactions 2 to 3 h
Ruminating 7 to 10 h
Drinking 30 min
Management activities 2.5 to 3.5 h

Importance of Nondietary Factors on Cow Productivity

A survey conducted on 47 farms in northeastern Spain demonstrated the significant effects of both stall availability and stall maintenance on the productivity of dairy cows (Bach et al., 2008). The mean stocking density of the study herds was 90% with a range of roughly 60% to 200%. There was a considerable amount of variation in productivity. Herds ranged from 20 to 34 kg/d (44 to 74 lb/d) of milk per cow, despite cows being fed the same ration that was mixed at a common location. Stall availability and stall maintenance explained approximately 40% of the observed differences in production. A positive relationship between stall availability indicated that each unit change in ratio of stalls-to-cows increased milk production by 7.5 kg (16.5 lb). However, there was no relationship between stall availability and milk production among the herds with stocking densities that did not exceed 100%, indicating that no benefit was gained by understocking. The results reported by Bach et al. (2008) are similar to those collected at the William H. Miner Agricultural Research Institute. Instead of evaluating production and stall availability, hours of rest and production were compared (Figure 1). Each hour increase in resting time resulted in a gain of 1.7 kg (3.7 lb) of milk production. Recent research (Fregonesi et al., 2007; Hill et al. 2007; Krawczel et al., 2008) has demonstrated the effect of stocking density, or stall availability, on resting time.

Effects of Stocking Density

General Effects

Much of the nondietary effects on productivity may be related to the behavioral effects of stocking density and the importance of each of these behaviors in maintaining health and well-being. In a broad sense, the effects of stocking density are driven by three key factors. First, the competition for resources limits the ability of cows to behave in a natural manner (Wechsler, 2007). The severity of the overstocking will dictate the extent that the natural feeding, resting, and rumination behaviors are inhibited. Each of those behaviors will be discussed in detail below.

In spite of the impact on behavior, there is a clear economic incentive for farmers to overcrowd free-stall facilities (Bewley et al., 2001), thereby spreading the fixed costs of production over a greater number of cows without expanding existing facilities. Additionally, the free-stall usage at stocking densities of 66% and 100% reported by Wagner-Storch et al. (2003) suggests that some overcrowding may be possible without affecting natural behavior. The extent that a barn can be overcrowded will depend on the social dynamic that exists within the herd as well as environmental factors.

Effects on Lying Behavior

The effects of stocking density on lying behavior have been of interest for several decades. The earliest research (Friend et al., 1977) suggested that total lying time was not affected until a stocking density greater than 150% was imposed. Lying time at the densities of 100%, 120%, and 150% was 14 h per day and was then reduced to 10 and 7 h per day when stocking density was increased to 200% and 300%, respectively. Neither of these stocking densities is realistic in on-farm situations, and more recent research has utilized stocking densities ranging from 67% to 150%, which are more representative of on-farm situations.

The lying behavior of cows subjected to either undercrowded (67% stocking density) or overcrowded (113%) conditions reported by Fregonesi and Leaver (2002) may explain why Bach et al. (2008) found no benefit to undercrowding. Average time per day spent lying (10 h), lying and ruminating (5 h), and occupying a stall (15 h) did not differ between the undercrowded and moderately overcrowded treatments. However, there were fewer aggressive interactions per hour in the undercrowded pen. These results suggest that there may not be a benefit to understocking cows beyond the dry and transition periods.

Conversely, stocking densities of 109%, 120%, 133%, and 150% resulted in a linear reduction of lying time, relative to 100% (Fregonesi et al., 2007). Cows spent 13 h per day lying at 100%, which was reduced by approximately 2 h when stocking density reached 150%. The latency to lie down was also reduced linearly. This was one of the first reports of stocking density altering stall usage. The reduction of latency to lie down when stocking density exceeded 120% may be misinterpreted as an increase in cow comfort and may actually pose an increased risk of environmental mastitis. Finally, a greater number of aggressive interactions per hour occurred with each increase in stocking density.

The reduction of lying time observed by Fregonesi et al. (2007) was similar to reduction in the percentage of stall usage in overcrowded cows when they were subjected to stocking densities of 100%, 113%, 131%, and 142% (Hill et al., 2007). The key differences between the work of Fregonesi et al. (2007) and Hill et al. (2007) were differences in group size (12 vs. 34) and overcrowding occurring at the stall only (Fregonesi et al., 2007) versus overcrowding at both the stall and the feed bunk (Hill et al., 2007). Some of the most dramatic effects of lying occurred between midnight and 4:00 a.m. Not only was this the time period that the greatest reduction in the percentage of cows lying occurred, but there was also a three-fold increase in the number of cows standing idly in the alley.

In general, the effects of overcrowding on lying are becoming the most well understood and predictable (Figure 2). An unpublished analysis of the relative response of lying in relation to stocking density observed in six different research trials by Grant (2008) demonstrated that the most pronounced effects occur above 120%.

Importance of Resting

The reduction of resting time by overcrowding is the most likely explanation of the reduction in performance associated with stall availability. The priority for rest over feeding was evident in a recent research trial. Cows were housed in pens containing an isolation area and a resource area for various portions of the day (Munksgaard et al., 2005). Within the resource area, cows were able to rest, feed, or socialize. As time in the resource area decreased, the portion of time spent lying increased in an effort to maintain a consistent number of hours of rest. Research at the Miner Institute demonstrated that overstocking results in an increasing percentage of cows standing idly waiting for access to freestalls (Hill et al., 2007). This effect becomes more pronounced between midnight and 4:00 a.m., the time period when the motivation to feed was reduced and the motivation to lie down increased. There is a potential for cows to spend more time waiting for a stall to become available than engaging in feeding over the course of a day. Finally, depriving cows of lying for a relatively limited period (2 to 4 hours), which is similar to those reported in a trial evaluating stocking density, resulted in cows attempting to recoup the lost resting time for the next 40 hours (Cooper et al., 2007). Routine management practices, such as herd health checks or freestall maintenance, could be sufficient to deprive cows of lying for this duration.

Beyond the effects on production, several important health-related factors are detrimentally altered by reduced lying time.

  1. The predominance of concrete flooring results in a greater strain on the hoof when cows are forced to stand for extended periods of time (Cook, 2002). The negative impact of the standing time is further worsened from the softening of the hoof by the manure slurry covering the alleyways, which leads to an increased probability of infections (Guard, 2002). These factors have been associated, either individually or in combination, with increased incidents of lameness.
  2. A stress response was evident in the concentration of cortisol in cows deprived of lying time (Munksgaard and Simonsen, 1996) relative to control cows with unrestricted ability to lie down. Increases in the concentration of cortisol are commonly associated with suppression of immune function.
  3.  Increased lying time also has a potential benefit for fetal growth. Significantly more blood flowed to the gravid uterine horn when cows were lying relative to when they were standing during several stages throughout the gestation period (Nishida et al., 2004).

Effects on Feeding

Although it is a highly variable relationship, overcrowding at the freestalls tends to result in overcrowding at the feed bunk. This relationship is highly dependent on the barn design (4-row versus 6-row) and severity of the freestall overcrowding. The effects of spatial allowance at the feed bunk of lactating dairy cows have been examined for the past three decades (Friend et al., 1977; DeVries et al., 2004; Huzzey et al., 2006). The earliest research established that reducing feed bunk space per cow to less than 10.6 cm (4 in.) per cow reduced feeding time (Friend et al., 1977). More recent research suggested that the effects of decreased feeding space were evident at a much greater space allotment than suggested by Friend et al. (1977). The behavioral effects of providing either 51 cm (20 in.), slightly less than the 60 cm (23 in.) commonly recommended, or 102 cm (40 in.) of bunk space per cow were examined by DeVries et al. (2004). Increasing the space allotment per cow to 102 cm (40 in.) reduced the number of aggressive interactions per cow and increased the percentage of cows feeding during the 90 minutes following the delivery of fresh total mixed ration. Similar results were found in a comparison of four stocking densities ranging from 75% to 300% (Huzzey et al., 2006). This study also evaluated the same stocking density in pens utilizing headlocks or a post-and-rail feed barrier. Feeding time decreased and aggression increased as stocking density increased. The effect became more pronounced with each increase in stocking density. Cows also spent more time standing idly waiting for access to feeding space. One potential coping strategy that was observed was the shift in feeding times, which may be problematic if the ration is sorted by the first cows to feed. The comparison of the two types of feed barriers found that the headlocks offered protection against displacements and reduced aggression at the feed bunk.

Effects on Rumination

Unlike lying and feeding behavior, conflicting effects on rumination from increased stocking density have been reported. Batchelder (2000) reported a significantly greater percentage of cows ruminating during the day at 100% stocking density compared to 130% (37% vs. 28%) on an average basis over 24 hours. In addition to differences in the average, the peak percentage of cows ruminating at any point (55%) was roughly 1.5 times greater than the peak percentage observed at 130% (32%). These results were not supported by the observations of Fregonesi and Leaver (2002). In addition to not finding any differences between under- and overcrowded pens in the time spent ruminating while lying, no differences in total time ruminating (8 h) were observed. There are two possible explanations for the difference in the observed effect. First, the Batchelder work was not replicated, so the results should be viewed with some skepticism. Second, the overcrowding (118%) imposed by Fregonesi and Leaver (2002) may have been insufficient to affect rumination. This is consistent with the main effects of overcrowding on lying becoming evident in cows housed at stocking densities above 120%.

Effect on Milk Quality

Unlike the associations between stall availability (Bach et al, 2008) or resting time (Grant, 2007) and productivity, a direct cause-and-effect relationship has not been found in designed research trials (Fregonesi and Leaver, 2002; Hill et al., 2007; and Krawczel et al., 2008b). Milk production has consistently been reported as unaffected. The duration of the experiment (1 or 2 weeks for Hill et al. [2007] and Krawczel et al. [2008b], respectively) or the number of animals used (Fregonesi and Leaver, 2002) may have contributed substantially to the inability of those experiments to detect changes in milk production. Milk quality, on the other hand, was affected by overcrowding (Hill et al., 2007). Milk fat (%) was reduced by approximately 0.2% at 142% compared to 100%. Somatic cell count (SCC) tended to increase above 113%. Supporting this increase in SCC was a numerical increase in the number of incidents of clinical mastitis per 305 cow-days at risk for cows housed at 142% compared to 100% (Krawczel et al., unpublished data, 2008). Although the number of clinical events was not statistically different, the 2.5-fold increase was an indication of the need for further research into the relationship between mastitis and stocking density.

Monitoring Cow Comfort in Overcrowded Pens

There are three commonly used indices of cow comfort utilized on commercial dairy farms for herd assessments.

  • The Cow Comfort Quotient (now more commonly called the Cow Comfort Index, CCI) is calculated by dividing the number of cows lying in a stall by the total number of cows in contact with a stall (Nelson, 1996). It was thought that this number would reflect the motivation of cows to enter and lie down in a stall.
  • The next index developed was the Stall Use Index (SUI), which sought to improve on the CCI by taking into account more cows within a pen (Overton et al., 2003). Stall Use Index is calculated by dividing the number of cows lying in a stall by the total number of eligible cows (defined as all cows not actively feeding) within the pen. Using all eligible cows was an attempt to incorporate all cows that were not engaged in productive behavior into the calculation of a pen’s comfort.
  • Cook et al. (2005) failed to find an association between CCI or SUI and total lying time. As a result, they suggested monitoring cow comfort using the portion of cows standing in a stall, referred to as the Stall Standing Index (SSI), which was associated with the total time cows spent standing in stalls. The work of Fregonesi et al. (2007) demonstrated that overcrowding decreases the latency to lie down after entering a stall. Similarly, Hill et al. (2007) demonstrated the substantial increase in the number of cows standing idly in alleyways waiting for an available stall during periods of peak stall usage (midnight to 4:00 a.m.). Between midnight and 4:00 a.m., the significant differences found in CCI did not suggest an influence of stocking density. The SUI decreased significantly above 113% with the value falling below the targeted benchmark of 0.75 (Overton et al., 2003) at the 142% stocking density.

Three conclusions were drawn from these data:

  1. First, the definition of cow comfort needed to be refined to include stall availability.
  2. Second, using this definition, SUI becomes the most appropriate index for assessing cow comfort due to the inclusion of cows standing idly in the alleys into its calculation.
  3. Finally, cow comfort should be assessed at a time when cows are most motivated to lie down.

Management factors, such as feeding and milking times, will determine the best time for individual farms to assess cow comfort. At the Miner Institute, feeding begins at 4:30 a.m. and milking at 4:00 a.m., 12:00 p.m., and 8:00 p.m. By midnight, feed has been available for approximately 20 hours, and most cows have had the opportunity to consume a post-milking meal. As a result, this marks the beginning of the period when cows are most motivated to lie down.

Summary of Responses to Stocking Density

The current understanding of how cows are affected by overcrowding can be separated into factors that have established responses and factors that require further investigation. The established responses are:

  • Reduced resting time
  • Increased idle standing in alleys
  • Altered feeding behavior
  • Greater aggression/displacements at the feed bunk
  • Cow comfort reflected by SUI

Those that need further investigation are:

  • Decreased rumination
  • Less milk
  • Lower milk fat
  • Greater SCC

Take-Home Messages on Overcrowding

  • To ensure that all cows obtain adequate rest, the design of the stalls and cow management should facilitate 10 to 14 hours of lying time for each cow within a pen. In addition, stalls should be sized to allow cows to assume all natural resting postures.
  • Feed bunk management should accommodate 5 hours per day of feeding time per cow. This availability should reduce aggressive interactions and prevent slug feeding.
  • Stocking density at the freestalls should not exceed 120%. The extent to which an individual farm can overcrowd will depend on its ability to meet the above resting and feeding requirements.
  • Further attention should be paid to first-calf heifers and lame cows that will be the most affected by overcrowding.
  • The total impact of stocking density will be dictated by the interaction of the stocking density, grouping strategies, and social interactions.

Author Information

Peter Krawczel
University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN

Rick Grant
William H. Miner Agricultural Research Institute, Chazy, NY

References

Bach, A., N. Valls, A. Solans, and T. Torrent. 2008. Associations between nondietary factors and dairy herd performance. J. Dairy Sci. 91:3259:3267.

Batchelder, T.L. 2000. The impact of head gates and overcrowding on production and behavior patterns of lactating dairy cows. Pages 325-330 in Dairy Housing and Equipment Systems: Managing and Planning for Profitability. NRAES Publ. 129. Camp Hill, PA.

Bewley, J., R.W. Palmer, and D.B. Jackson-Smith. 2001. An overview of experiences of Wisconsin dairy farmers who modernized their operations. J. Dairy Sci. 84:717-729.

Cook, N.B. 2002b. The influence of barn design on dairy cow hygiene, lameness, and udder health. Pages 97-103 in Proc. 35th Annu. Conf. Am. Assoc. Bovine Pract., Stillwater, OK.

Cook, N.B., T.B. Bennett, and K.V. Nordlund. 2005. Monitoring indices of cow comfort in free-stall-housed dairy herds. J. Dairy Sci. 88:3876-3885.

Cooper, M.D., D.R. Arney, and C.J.C. Phillips. 2007. Two- or four-hour lying deprivation on the lying behavior of lactating dairy cows. J. Dairy Sci. 90:1149-1158.

DeVries, T.J., M.A.G. von Keyserlingk, and D.M. Weary. 2004. Effect of feeding space on the inter-cow distance, aggression, and feeding behavior of free-stall housed lactating dairy cows. J. Dairy Sci. 87:1432-1438.

Fregonesi, J.A., and J.D. Leaver. 2002. Influence of space allowance and milk yield level on behaviour, performance and health of dairy cows housed in straw yard and cubicle systems. Livest. Prod. Sci. 78:245-257.

Fregonesi, J.A., C.B. Tucker, and D.M. Weary. 2007. Overstocking reduces lying time in dairy cows. J. Dairy Sci. 90:3349-3354.

Friend, T.H., C.E. Polan, and M.L. McGilliard. 1977. Free stall and feed bunk requirements relative to behavior, production, and individual feed intake in dairy cows. J. Dairy Sci. 60:108-116.

Grant, R. 2007. Taking advantage of natural behavior improves dairy cow performance. Pages 225-236 in Proc. Western Dairy Management Conf., Reno, NV.

Grant, R.J., and J.L. Albright. 2001. Effect of animal grouping on feeding behavior and intake of dairy cattle. J. Dairy Sci. 84:E156-E163.

Guard, C. 2002. Environmental risk factors contributing to lameness in dairy cattle. Pages 271-277 in Dairy Housing and Equipment Systems, Managing, and Planning for Profitability. Natural Resource, Agriculture, and Engineering Service Publ. 129. Camp Hill, PA.

Hill, C.T., P.D. Krawczel, H.M. Dann, C.S. Ballard, R.C. Hovey, and R.J. Grant. 2007. Effect of stocking density on the short-term behavior of dairy cows. J. Dairy Sci. 90 (Suppl. 1):244.

Huzzey, J.M., T.J. DeVries, P. Valois, and M.A.G. von Keyserlingk. 2006. Stocking density and feed barrier design affect the feeding and social behavior of dairy cattle. J. Dairy Sci. 89:126-133.

Krawczel, P.D., C.T. Hill, H.M. Dann, and R.J. Grant. 2008a. Short communication: Effect of stocking density on indices of cow comfort. J. Dairy Sci. 91:1903-1907.

Krawczel, P.D., C.S. Mooney, H.M. Dann, M.P. Carter, R.E. Butzler, C.S. Ballard, and R.J. Grant. 2008b. Effect of alternative models for increasing stocking density on the lying behavior, hygiene, and short-term productivity of lactating Holstein dairy cattle. J. Dairy Sci. 91 (Suppl.):401

Matzke, W.C., and R.J. Grant. 2002. Behavior of primi- and multiparous lactating dairy cattle in commingled groups. J. Dairy Sci. 85(Suppl. 1):372. (Abstr.)

Munksgaard, L., M.B. Jensen, L.J. Pedersen, S.W. Hansen, and L. Matthews. 2005. Quantifying behavioral priorities: Effects of time constraints on the behavior of dairy cows, Bos taurus. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 92:3-14.

Munksgaard, L., and H.B. Simonsen. 1996. Behavioral and pituitary adrenal-axis responses of dairy cows to social isolation and deprivation of lying down. J. Anim. Sci. 74:769-778.

Nelson, A.J. 1996. On-farm nutrition diagnostics. Pages 76-85 in Proc. 29th Annu. Conf. Am. Bovine Pract., San Diego, CA. Am. Assoc. Bovine Pract., Rome, GA.

Nishida, T., K. Hosoda, H. Matsuyama, and M. Ishida. 2004. Effect of lying behavior on uterine blood flow during the third trimester of gestation. J. Dairy Sci. 87:2388-2392.

Overton, M.W., D.A. Moore, and W.M. Sischo. 2003. Comparison of commonly used indices to evaluate dairy cattle lying behavior. Pages 125-130 in Proc. 5th Int. Dairy Housing Conf., Fort Worth, TX. Am. Soc. Agric. Biol. Eng., St. Joseph, MI.

Wagner-Storch, A.M., R.W. Palmer, and D.W. Kammel. 2003. Factors affecting stall use for different free-stall bases. J. Dairy Sci. 86:2253-2266.

Wechsler, B. 2007. Normal behaviour as a basis for animal welfare assessment. Anim. Welf. 16:107-110.

Weirenga, H.K., and H. Hopster. 1990. The significance of cubicles for the behaviour of dairy cows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 26:309-337.

Winkler, C., C.B. Tucker, and D.M. Weary. 2003. Effects of stall availability on time budgets and agonistic interactions in dairy cattle. Page 130 in Proc. 37th Inter. Cong. of the ISAE. Abano Terme, Italy. (Abstr.)